Newsletter Subject

An almost unbearably lovely vintage illustrated ode to friendship, the cognitive science and moral philosophy of whether your dog loves you, and more

From

brainpickings.org

Email Address

newsletter@brainpickings.org

Sent On

Sun, Sep 8, 2019 01:12 PM

Email Preheader Text

NOTE: This newsletter might be cut short by your email program. . If a friend forwarded it to you

NOTE: This newsletter might be cut short by your email program. [View it in full](.  If a friend forwarded it to you and you'd like your very own newsletter, [subscribe here]( — it's free.  Need to modify your subscription? You can [change your email address]( or [unsubscribe](. [Brain Pickings]( [Welcome] Dear {NAME}, welcome to this week's edition of the [brainpickings.org]( newsletter by Maria Popova. If you missed last week's digest — 20-year-old Lord Byron's moving elegy for his beloved dog, an enchanting journey to the hidden universe below our feet, America's first woman mapmaker — you can catch up [right here](. And if you are enjoying this labor of love, please consider supporting it with a [donation]( – I spend innumerable hours and tremendous resources on it each week, and every little bit of support helps enormously. If you already donate: THANK YOU. [I Like You: An Almost Unbearably Lovely Vintage Illustrated Ode to Friendship]( [ilikeyou_stoddard.jpg?fit=320%2C364]( “Ponder for a long time whether you shall admit a given person to your friendship,” Seneca counseled two millennia ago in his [timeless meditation on true and false friendship]( “but when you have decided to admit him, welcome him with all your heart and soul.” I often ponder friendship — that crowning glory of life — and the strain of protecting its sanctity from [the commodification of the word “friend”]( in this age of social media. Adrienne Rich exposed the naked heart of it in her bittersweet assertion that [“we can count on so few people to go that hard way with us.”]( I side with astronomer Maria Mitchell in that the few who do accompany us intimately along the walk of life [shape who we become]( and with poet and philosopher David Whyte in that [“all friendships of any length are based on a continued, mutual forgiveness.”]( But what, really, is the meaning and measure of friendship? Like most things of beauty, it is slippery to define yet deeply felt. Paradoxically, devastatingly, it is often recognized most acutely [through its sudden loss](. It lives most intimately not in the grand gestures but in the littlest things that add up, in the final calculus of life, to the bigness of any true bond. That is what children’s book author Sandol Stoddard and illustrator Jacqueline Chwast explore with immense sweetness and sensitivity in the 1965 gem [I Like You]( ([public library]( — one of the tenderest and most touching presents I’ve ever gotten, from one of my dearest friends, and the platonic-love counterpart to Elizabeth Barrett Browning’s classic romantic-love sonnet [“How Do I Love Thee?”]( [ilikeyou1.jpg?resize=680%2C781]( [ilikeyou2.jpg?resize=680%2C778]( [ilikeyou3.jpg?resize=680%2C789]( Stoddard — who wrote more than twenty children’s books and [the first major book]( advocating for human-centric end-of-life care, lived to be 90, and died the mother of five children, ten grandchildren, and ten great-grandchildren — was once asked to identify the underlying theme across all of her books. She answered simply, “Love.” And love — that sweetest, most knotless and untroubled kind — is what radiates from these simple, surprisingly profound verse-like meditations on friendship, illustrated with the kindred sensibility of Chwast’s simple yet richly expressive black-and-white line drawings. [ilikeyou4.jpg?resize=680%2C792]( [ilikeyou5.jpg?resize=680%2C782]( [ilikeyou6.jpg?resize=680%2C785]( [ilikeyou8.jpg?resize=680%2C792]( [ilikeyou9.jpg?resize=680%2C780]( [ilikeyou10.jpg?resize=680%2C787]( [ilikeyou11.jpg?resize=680%2C792]( [ilikeyou12.jpg?resize=680%2C796]( Published the same year as [Love Is Walking Hand in Hand]( — that charming catalogue of little moments that define love, channeled by the Charlie Brown, Lucy, and the rest of the Peanuts — the book confers upon friendship the delight and dignity we tend to reserve, foolishly so, for romantic love only. [ilikeyou13.jpg?resize=680%2C794]( [ilikeyou17.jpg?resize=680%2C785]( [ilikeyou14.jpg?resize=680%2C809]( [ilikeyou15.jpg?resize=680%2C778]( [ilikeyou16.jpg?resize=680%2C818]( [ilikeyou20.jpg?resize=680%2C780]( More than half a century later, [I Like You]( remains a timeless treasure, as delicious to give and as it is to receive. Complement it with Little Prince author Antoine de Saint-Exupéry on [losing a friend]( and Kahlil Gibran on [the building blocks of meaningful connection]( then revisit two other charming picture-books about friendship from the same era: Ruth Krauss’s infinitely delightful [I’ll Be You and You Be Me]( illustrated by the young Maurice Sendak, and Janice May Urdy’s clever reverse-psychology gem [Let’s Be Enemies]( also illustrated by Sendak, just as he was beginning to dream up Where the Wild Things Are. [Forward to a friend]( Online]( [Like on Facebook]( donating=loving I pour tremendous time, thought, heart, and resources into Brain Pickings, which remains free and ad-free, and is made possible by patronage. If you find any joy, stimulation, and consolation in my labor of love, please consider supporting it with a donation. And if you already donate, from the bottom of my heart: THANK YOU. monthly donation You can become a Sustaining Patron with a recurring monthly donation of your choosing, between a cup of tea and a Brooklyn lunch.  one-time donation Or you can become a Spontaneous Supporter with a one-time donation in any amount. [Start Now](  [Give Now]( [Does Your Dog Really Love You and What Does That Really Mean? A Journey in Cognitive Science and Moral Philosophy]( [ourdogsourselves_horowitz.jpg?fit=320%2C483]( That humans love their dogs is a fundamental fact of our animal heart, as indisputable and irrepealable as gravity — just look at Lord Byron’s leaden [eulogy for his beloved dog](. But whether our dogs “love” us and what that really means is a question that hurls the human heart into perennial restlessness, oscillating between absolute, arrogant certainty and endless, insecure doubt. Its answer hints at the elemental nature of all emotion, at the central puzzlement of consciousness, at the very meaning of love, and at the unnerving fact that we can never fully know the inner life of another, be they human or other animal. That question is what cognitive scientist [Alexandra Horowitz]( director of the Dog Cognition Lab at Barnard College, explores in a chapter of her altogether fascinating book [Our Dogs, Ourselves: The Story of a Singular Bond]( ([public library](. [stella_10x8.jpg?resize=680%2C544] Horowitz notes that, both in her lab and while observing dogs in the urban wild, she constantly sees behaviors from which we instinctively infer human-like emotions — curiosity when a dog faces a dancing robot, surprise when a hidden researcher emerges from behind a door — and yet she is frequently asked whether dogs are really capable of the most sweeping human emotions: love, anger, ennui. Are we right to imagine [“If I could I would bite every sorrow until it fled”]( into a beloved dog’s mental monologue? Framing these questions as “a testament to both the ardor of our interest in our dogs, and our uncertainty about the dog’s experience,” Horowitz writes: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]As our own days may be colored with anxiety, anticipation, or foreboding — are dogs’ days so colored? As we respond to events and people with empathy, sarcasm, or incredulity — do dogs tend toward such sentiments? Many of these questions boil down to whether dogs have feelings or emotions at all. But of course they do. Look at it adaptively: emotions are messaging to the muscles and response system to circumvent the closed-door discussions between the sensory organs and brain. I see a tiger; I know that tigers are predators and this one is coming toward me . . . and Hey!, chimes the brain emotively, Be afraid! Run! Look at it neurologically: the areas of human brains that are active when we feel, sigh, yearn, and despair are also found in dogs’ brains. Look at it behaviorally: though we are not always great at naming which behavior indicates what emotion (as we will shortly see), the wide array of different behaviors and postures of dogs tells us about their internal states. Look at it sensibly. The alternative to having emotions — having undifferentiated experience — defies reason, defies Darwin, defies continuity. Human emotions did not emerge mysteriously and fully formed out of unfeeling automata. Keep in mind that the last popular advocate of the latter belief, Descartes, lived in a time when bloodletting was still considered salubrious. [mairakalman_beloveddog16.jpg] Art by Maira Kalman from [Beloved Dog]( But while the question of whether dogs feel is a fossil of hubristic medievalism, the question of what and how dogs feel remains just on the cusp of our ability to answer — for our answers are mired in our own projections. After all, the [qualia]( of any conscious experience is singular to the consciousness having it and impenetrable to other consciousnesses — Nina Simone serenaded the impossibility of precisely knowing the qualia of another human animal when she sang “I wish you could know what it means to be me,” let alone the qualia of a non-human animal. And yet we presume to easily read a dog’s feeling states. A century and a half after Darwin wrote that “man himself cannot express love and humility by external signs, so plainly as does a dog, when with drooping ears, hanging lips, flexuous body, and wagging tail, he meets his beloved master,” Horowitz pulls into question the plainness of emotional inferences drawn from behavioral cues. Having previously written beautifully about [how a walk with her own dog ignited an awareness of the myriad different ways of experiencing the same reality]( she considers the difference between description and emotional diagnosis: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]As shorthand, it makes good sense to me to use emotional terms to describe what I’m seeing. In the lab, I would more likely say, The dog’s head extends forward, leading the body by an extra half-step; the ears are perked into their full height (read: curiosity). A dog jumps back, preparing the body for escape; a “rurf” sound slips out (surprise). Retreating, the dog’s body shrinks down and back (anxiety); on approach, a dog pulls away her head, lifts her paw, curls her lip (disgust); with a high, loosely wagging tail, the dog leaps with two or four legs and attempts to lick every nearby face, dog or human (delight). I don’t use those shorthand words as my first descriptions of what they are doing — because I hesitate to assume that a dog’s experience of what looks like curiosity or delight is precisely like mine. While the similarities across mammalian brains make it highly likely that all mammals have diverse emotional experiences, we all also have very different lived experiences, based on, for humans, our cultures, where we live, and the people we meet. So, too, for dogs. My own guess is that, planted into a dog’s body, we wouldn’t recognize the feelings we’re flooded with as being just like our own. But that there are feelings, I’ve no doubt. In this way, I inhabit the territory between the presumptive granting of subjective experience just like humans — and complete denial of any experience. Not presuming to know the dog’s subjective experience is not at all the same as denying them any experience at all. [mairakalman_beloveddog17.jpg] Art by Maira Kalman from [Beloved Dog]( Paradoxically, she points out, denial has been the crucible of the scientific study of animal consciousness — with strikingly cruel consequences that gnaw at the foundations of morality: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Without definitive evidence of an animal’s fear of pain, researchers say, how can we be sure that the animal feels fear — or pain — at all? Weirdly, most of the history of medical and psychiatric research has also seemed not to doubt the reality of animals’ feelings. In fact, it presumes feelings in its very premise. To prove the efficacy of an anti-anxiety drug for humans, the drug first has to be roundly vetted on an “animal model”: essentially, lab animals have to be made anxious, then given the test, and have their anxiety dissipate (while no other ill effects arise). A history of this kind of thinking is written between the lines of every medical study using animals: they are so similar to us, thus they are a good model for humans. […] Should someone make the claim to me that a dog definitely can’t be “depressed,” or benefit from anti-depression medication, I’ll take their hand and walk them back in time. Several decades ago, depression research took a step forward with the development of the “learned helplessness” model, made famous by Martin Seligman. He and his colleague came up with a scheme to see if helplessness could be induced by circumstance. Brace yourself: it involved dogs. In a passage difficult to read without growing heavy-hearted and fiery with anger, Horowitz goes on to summarize the classic behavioral psychology study — an experiment that involved thirty-two “adult mongrel dogs” who never smelled the outdoor air and lived enslaved in the lab, where they were strapped down and assaulted by electric shocks and 70-decibel noise until they “learned” that they were utterly helpless. Horowitz confesses in a footnote that she had to read the study in three harrowing sittings, punctuated by slamming her computer shut and leaving the room. (Her own lab keeps no live animals, though there are two stuffed toy-dogs, both affectionately named by the researchers. Volunteer subjects come from the “real” world, including one human-canine duo who traveled 210 miles to participate in a 30-minute study.) She reflects on the grim morale of Seligman’s study: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Dogs were shocked, driven to depression and passivity and impotence, to prove that we could feel passivity and impotent in depression. Dogs are still widely used in medical research, make no mistake: this is happening now. Also now. And again. […] To watch struggling animals without working to relieve their struggle demonstrates the great dissociation we condone with animals. Our society’s attitude toward animals is thus mismatched. We grant them feelings when it suits our testing needs, but grant them no feelings when it would not suit our testing needs. The human behavior in these test settings — electrocuting; near-drowning — is considered animal cruelty anywhere outside of the test setting. So why is the question of animal emotions still posed? We are trapped on the far reaches of the pendulum’s swing: either assuming dogs are entirely unlike us or assuming dogs are just like us. As wrongheaded as it is to presume dogs to be unfeeling, it is no more correct to presumptively grant them a humanlike emotional life. (Nor must it be somewhere in-between: for all we know, dogs’ emotional experience is far more elaborate than ours.) We glance at dogs and conclude we know what they’re feeling, but our haste to make such conjecture on little evidence — and inability to read a dog’s emotions when they are displayed — is profound. [chance.jpg?resize=680%2C907] Curiously, while we are poor readers of a dog’s emotions, dogs seem to be excellent readers of ours. One of the fascinating findings of Horowitz’s lab is that the familiar “guilty look” we so often perceive in dogs — tail tucked, head lowered, eyebrows slightly knit — is not an indication of a dog’s guilt over a misbehavior but of having registered that the owner is angry or about to get angry, independent of whether or not the dog has done something guilt-worthy. Similarly, Horowitz’s lab found that what classic behavioral studies of fairness perception — one dog is given more treats, another fewer — have interpreted as “jealousy” is simply a dog’s “reasonable refusal to work for nothing.” Her experiment also illuminates the lovely eternal optimism of the dog’s nature: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Against expectation, they preferred to hang out with the unfair person. Again, it seems like they are motivated less by the kinds of feelings of unfairness or jealousy that humans have than by pure optimism that maybe this time, some of those treats will be tossed their way… Lurking beneath all the ambiguity, affect-blindness, and projection is a testament to the great Buddhist teacher Thich Nhat Hanh’s assertion that [“understanding is love’s other name.”]( Horowitz considers the intimate crux of our difficulty in discerning dogs’ emotions: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Our inability to read dogs’ emotions well probably begins with our inability to understand our own emotions well. Though perfectly accessible to us — and only to us, truly — our society is constantly putting us to work to “get in touch with” our emotions. And that’s when they are right there for the touching. Given our difficulty, it’s no wonder we are ill-equipped to figure out the emotions of the four-legged creature beside us. So we default to granting dogs emotions, but of the most human sort. We assume dogs are not only in the room with us, but sharing a kind of hive mind with humans. […] Does your dog love you? Watch them, and you tell me. [mairakalman_beloveddog4.jpg] Art by Maira Kalman from [Beloved Dog]( Caroline Paul, writing about another beloved four-legged species of companion, summed up the central paradox of human-pet emotional understanding — and of any emotional understanding — perfectly: [“You can never know anyone as completely as you want. But that’s okay, love is better.”]( Complement the thoroughly wonderful and revelatory [Our Dogs, Ourselves]( with artist Maria Kalman’s [illustrated love letter to dogs]( and John Homans’s beautiful and bittersweet [canine-inspired meditation on love, loss, and the art of presence]( then revisit Horowitz on [how dogs actually “see” the world through smell]( and [what they can teach us about accessing the hidden layers of reality](. [Forward to a friend]( Online]( [Like on Facebook]( [Advice to a Daughter from Pioneering Political Philosopher and Feminism Founding Mother Mary Wollstonecraft]( [maria_marywollstonecraft.jpg?fit=252%2C400]( Six years after Mary Wollstonecraft (April 27, 1759–September 10, 1797) composed her epoch-making 1792 treatise [Vindication of the Rights of Woman]( which became the foundation of what we today call feminism, she fell in love with the radical political philosopher William Godwin. The two forged [the original marriage of equals]( and conceived a daughter — future Frankenstein author [Mary Wollstonecraft Shelley](. Ten days after giving birth to baby Mary, Wollstonecraft died at only thirty eight, leaving behind the foundation for the next two centuries of humanity’s model of gender equality, and a half-orphaned baby daughter who would come to know her mother through her writing. [marywollstonecraft_johnopie.jpg?resize=680%2C829] Mary Wollstonecraft shortly before her death. Portrait by John Opie. In the final years of her life, Wollstonecraft had begun working on [Maria; or The Wrongs of Woman]( ([free ebook]( | [public library]( — a philosophical novel intended as a sequel to Vindication and laced with strong autobiographical strands, exploring subjects like slavery, class, marriage, motherhood, female desire, dignity, and the wellspring of agency. Unlike the astonishing rapidity with which Wollstonecraft the political philosopher had composed her humanist treatises, Wollstonecraft the literary artist struggled to complete the novel, doing more research for it than for any of her nonfiction. Godwin would later recall that “she was sensible how arduous a task it is to produce a truly excellent novel; and she roused her faculties to grapple with it.” Before she could finish the manuscript, Wollstonecraft died of complications from childbirth — a devastatingly common killer of women for the vast majority of human history. Godwin published the novel a year later, as part of a collection of Wollstonecraft’s posthumous works. Their daughter, who learned to read partly by tracing the letters on Wollstonecraft’s gravestone, would spend the rest of her life trying to get to know her mother through her work, of which Maria was in many ways the most personal. [blake_maryshelley.jpg?resize=552%2C696] “The Child Mary Shelley (at her Mother’s Death)” by [William Blake]( One particular passage from the seventh chapter, chillingly prescient given Wollstonecraft’s fate, would endure for Mary as the sage and empowering life-advice her mother never lived to give her: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Death may snatch me from you, before you can weigh my advice, or enter into my reasoning: I would then, with fond anxiety, lead you very early in life to form your grand principle of action, to save you from the vain regret of having, through irresolution, let the spring-tide of existence pass away, unimproved, unenjoyed. — Gain experience — ah! gain it — while experience is worth having, and acquire sufficient fortitude to pursue your own happiness; it includes your utility, by a direct path. What is wisdom too often, but the owl of the goddess, who sits moping in a desolated heart. In consonance with E.E. Cummings’s invigorating wisdom on [the courage to be yourself]( Wollstonecraft adds: [2e292385-dc1c-4cfe-b95e-845f6f98c2ec.png]Always appear what you are, and you will not pass through existence without enjoying its genuine blessings, love and respect. Complement with Maya Angelou’s [letter to the daughter she never had]( and W.E.B. Du Bois’s [magnificent life-advice to the daughter he did have]( then revisit Wollstonecraft on [loneliness and the courage of unwavering affection]( her [stirring love letters]( to and from Godwin, and her [moral primer for children]( illustrated by William Blake. [Forward to a friend]( Online]( [Like on Facebook]( donating=loving I pour tremendous time, thought, heart, and resources into Brain Pickings, which remains free and ad-free, and is made possible by patronage. If you find any joy, stimulation, and consolation in my labor of love, please consider supporting it with a donation. And if you already donate, from the bottom of my heart: THANK YOU. monthly donation You can become a Sustaining Patron with a recurring monthly donation of your choosing, between a cup of tea and a Brooklyn lunch.  one-time donation Or you can become a Spontaneous Supporter with a one-time donation in any amount. [Start Now](  [Give Now]( [---] You're receiving this email because you subscribed on Brain Pickings. This weekly newsletter comes out on Sundays and offers the week's most unmissable articles. Brain Pickings NOT A MAILING ADDRESS 159 Pioneer StreetBrooklyn, NY 11231 [Add us to your address book]( [unsubscribe from this list](   [update subscription preferences](

EDM Keywords (388)

yet year wrote wrongs wrongheaded written would worth world work wonder women woman wollstonecraft wish wisdom whether wellspring welcome weirdly weigh week way watch want walk vindication utility use us unfeeling unfairness understanding understand uncertainty two true treats trapped tracing touch tossed time tigers tiger thinking things testament test territory tenderest tend tell tea task take sweetest sure sundays summarize suits suit subscription subscribed study strapped strain story soul somewhere society smell slippery slamming singular simply similar side shorthand sharing sequel sensitivity sensibly sensible sendak seligman seeing see scheme save sang sanctity sage roused room rights right revelatory rest respond resources research remains relieve registered reflects recognize receiving reasoning really reality read radiates questions question qualia pursue prove protecting projections projection produce presuming presume presence premise preferred predators postures points poet planted plainness plainly perked people pendulum peanuts patronage passivity pass participate part pain owner owl one often offers novel nothing note never neurologically naming must muscles mother modify model mistake misbehavior mired mind messaging meets meet medical measure means meaning maybe mary maria man mammals make love losing look loneliness lives live lines like life letters letter length leaving learned laced labor lab know knotless kinds kind journey jealousy irrepealable intimately interpreted interest inhabit induced indisputable indication incredulity includes inability impotent impotence impossibility impenetrable imagine illustrated identify hurls humility humans humanity human horowitz history hesitate heart haste happiness happening hang hand half guilt guess gravity grapple grant godwin goddess go gnaw glance given give get full friendships friendship friend foundations foundation fossil forward form foreboding footnote flooded fled find figure fiery fell feelings feeling fear far faculties fact experiment experiencing experience expectation events escape equals enter enjoying emotions emotion email elaborate efficacy edition ears early dream doubt door donation dogs dog displayed dignity difficulty difference died development despair description describe depression depressed denying denial delight delicious default decided death daughter cusp cup cultures crucible course courage count could correct consonance consolation considers consciousness conjecture condone conclude conceived composed complications completely complete commodification colored collection claim circumvent chwast choosing children childbirth chapter change century catch brain bottom books body bloodletting bigness benefit behind beginning become became beauty beautiful based back awareness attempts assume assertion assaulted asked art areas arduous ardor approach answers answer another animals animal angry alternative also age advice admit add acutely active action accessing ability 90

Marketing emails from brainpickings.org

View More
Sent On

25/09/2024

Sent On

01/09/2024

Sent On

21/08/2024

Sent On

18/08/2024

Sent On

14/08/2024

Sent On

11/08/2024

Email Content Statistics

Subscribe Now

Subject Line Length

Data shows that subject lines with 6 to 10 words generated 21 percent higher open rate.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Words

The more words in the content, the more time the user will need to spend reading. Get straight to the point with catchy short phrases and interesting photos and graphics.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Images

More images or large images might cause the email to load slower. Aim for a balance of words and images.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Time to Read

Longer reading time requires more attention and patience from users. Aim for short phrases and catchy keywords.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Predicted open rate

Subscribe Now

Spam Score

Spam score is determined by a large number of checks performed on the content of the email. For the best delivery results, it is advised to lower your spam score as much as possible.

Subscribe Now

Flesch reading score

Flesch reading score measures how complex a text is. The lower the score, the more difficult the text is to read. The Flesch readability score uses the average length of your sentences (measured by the number of words) and the average number of syllables per word in an equation to calculate the reading ease. Text with a very high Flesch reading ease score (about 100) is straightforward and easy to read, with short sentences and no words of more than two syllables. Usually, a reading ease score of 60-70 is considered acceptable/normal for web copy.

Subscribe Now

Technologies

What powers this email? Every email we receive is parsed to determine the sending ESP and any additional email technologies used.

Subscribe Now

Email Size (not include images)

Font Used

No. Font Name
Subscribe Now

Copyright © 2019–2025 SimilarMail.