Newsletter Subject

How Farmers Use Bees to Scare Away Elephants

From

nautil.us

Email Address

newsletters@nautil.us

Sent On

Thu, Oct 26, 2023 11:04 AM

Email Preheader Text

Plus: The latest from Nautilus, and this week’s Facts So Romantic. | Did a friend forward this?

Plus: The latest from Nautilus, and this week’s Facts So Romantic. [View in browser](| [Join Nautilus]( Did a friend forward this? [Subscribe here.]( This Thursday, your free member newsletter includes one full story, by the environmental journalist Charles Digges. After that, be sure to check out this week’s Facts So Romantic. [ENVIRONMENT]( Elephants Are Total Scaredy-Cats Around Bees The buzz on preventing elephants from plundering communities. BY CHARLES DIGGES Is there any kind of fence that can make humans and elephants good neighbors? It’s a question Dominique Gonçalves has had to ponder as she leads the elephant ecology project at Mozambique’s Gorongosa National Park, which is not surrounded by a physical barrier. A number [of pioneering studies throughout Sub-Saharan]( Africa over the past several years showed a solution that was simple and natural: bees. As it turns out, the tiny, ubiquitous honeybee has the power to terrify a mammal that’s 22 million times its size. In fact, even the sound of the insect’s buzz is enough to send a family of elephants into a panic, showed [studies by Lucy King](, an Oxford zoologist and preeminent researcher in human-elephant coexistence at the nonprofit [Save the Elephants](. Upon hearing the telltale hum, elephants will run, kick up dust, shake their heads as if trying to swat the bees out of the air, trumpeting distressed warnings to other elephants as they flee. Of course, a bee’s stinger can’t penetrate the thick hide of an elephant. But when bees swarm—and African bees swarm aggressively—hundreds of bees might sting an elephant in its most sensitive areas, like the trunk, the mouth, and eyes. And it hurts. The bee has the power to terrify a mammal that’s 22 million times its size. Building on King’s insights, Paola Branco of the University of Idaho conducted a massive two-year-long experiment in Gorongosa that culminated [in a 2019 paper]( she co-authored with King, Marc Stalmans, Gorongosa’s director of scientific services, Princeton zoologist Robert Pringle, and others. Their research aimed to settle tensions between human farmers and the park’s growing population of marauding pachyderms—with the help of bees. Although elephants are peaceful by nature, they can and will trample grain, swipe crops, topple down silos, and knock down entire houses. Given half a chance, elephants from the fenceless sprawl of Gorongosa in the Lower Rift Valley will steal across the Pungwe River—which acts as the southern border between the million-acre park and the rest of rural Mozambique—stomping into villages in search of a meal. Humans share blame in the squabble. Natural habitats for elephants are rapidly being tilled into croplands, encroaching on food sources, often leaving the animals little choice but to ransack and steal. And while the population of African elephants has been precipitously dropping, the number of humans in Sub-Saharan Africa continues to skyrocket, rising from 930 million in 2012 to 1.2 billion in 2022, [data from the World Bank show.]( The result is that elephants and people are often competing for the same resources. The animals—which typically eat about 300 pounds of vegetation a day—can decimate an entire farm’s harvest overnight. In a way, it’s not a terrible problem to have, says Gonçalves, who grew up in the town of Beira, a few hours away from Gorongosa. The Mozambican civil war, which raged from 1977 to 1992, saw 95 percent of Gorongosa’s large animals killed. The [impact on elephants]( was especially profound. Slaughtered by warring troops who traded their tusks for more guns, the park’s pre-war population of elephants dwindled from 4,000 to only about 200 by the time the conflict ended. Thirty years on, says Gonçalves, that population is bouncing back and now numbers a little more than 1,000. The crop raids are a side effect of that recovery. But straying elephants cause havoc for small-scale subsistence farmers in Gorongosa’s buffer zone, the liminal area that stretches around the 1,500 square miles of the park and is home to more than 200,000 people. The more elephant numbers bounce back, the more of them there are to go on nighttime ransacking missions. “It’s a question of how humans and elephants are overlapping,” says Gonçalves. “If there are agricultural plots, that can create situations of conflict, when elephants either eat or trample or destroy. That has a huge economic security impact for farmers.” BUZZ OFF: Buckets of bees are strung on a fence wire along a river in Mozambique. When elephants trip the wire, bees swarm out of the boxes. The elephants hightail back to where they came from. Courtesy of Gorongosa National Park. Most subsistence farmers don’t have the resources to surround their plots with expensive wire fences, and often must resort to [less effective deterrents]( like banging sheet metal to scare elephants away, burning tires to produce acrid smoke, or lying in wait in the bushes at night with flashlights and fireworks to startle the animals. These confrontations can prove lethal for both sides. In [July of 2022](, five people harvesting their crops in the Mozambican province of Cabo Delgado—870 miles northeast of Gorongosa—were trampled by elephants from the Quirimbas National Park. On occasion, humans retaliate. In Kenya, for instance, wildlife authorities shoot between 50 and 120 elephants per year. “That’s the worst-case scenario,” Gonçalves says. “Both people and elephants end up being dead.” So she and her colleagues at Gorongosa decided to intervene to keep the two sides happy. It began, as many science things do, with an experiment. In 2017, researchers strung a series of fences at well-trafficked elephant crossing points along the Pungwe River. Some of the fences relied on methods of elephant deterrent already popular among small-scale farmers, such as twine soaked in chilis.1 But between others, the researchers ran bailing twine from which they suspended hives populated by the [famously irascible]( African bee, a species nearly identical to its European and North American cousins—but for its more aggressive tendencies. Still others combined chili-coated twine and bee hives. The researchers then tranquilized and fitted 12 male elephants—males being more apt to forage in croplands—with GPS collars. Satellite data pinged from the collars, combined with the observations of local community members, allowed the researchers to keep exacting tabs on where the elephants wandered and whether the experimental fences prevented them from stumbling into neighboring farmsteads to rummage. Sure enough, the fences worked. The chili fences reduced elephant river crossings by 80 percent—while the beehive fences thwarted a whopping 95 percent of cross-river forays by the animals. (Beehives strung on twine coated in chilis were, surprisingly, the least effective. King and her co-authors surmised that the coating weighed the twine down, making it easier for elephants to simply step over it.) Cameras placed near fences involving beehives showed that the elephants would trip the bailing twine, thus shaking the suspended beehives. Immediately, this would stir the bees to action, and they would swarm and mount an attack. In footage taken at night, shadowy silhouettes of peeved elephants [can be seen]( turning tail and lolloping in unison out of the frame. Beehive fences thwarted a whopping 95 percent of forays by the animals. The true beauty of this method, write Branco and her coauthors in the study, is that it allows discontinuous fencing to block key corridors used by elephants as they go on crop raids rather than fencing individual farms or entire nature preservers, like Gorongosa—which both for the farmers and the park could prove prohibitively expensive, running into the thousands and even tens of thousands of dollars. To build 15 hives and string them from posts, Branco and her team spent a total of $773. The results at Gorongosa jibed with those from [another field study]( that King conducted in Kenya in 2017. In that experiment, researchers strung beehive fences around 10 farms that were located near a nature preserve, reducing elephant raids by 80 percent. Bee fences have also been found to be an effective deterrent against crop raiding Asian elephants as well, showed [a 2018 study]( King conducted in Sri Lanka. In that case, merely playing recordings of angry bees through speakers in the field was enough to cause the elephants to flee. Gonçalves says the bee barriers set an important precedent for future conservation efforts by offering a nonlethal method of control that doesn’t create divisions between the park and the people who live near it. Bee fences alone aren’t enough. Other animals that are less flustered by bees, such as baboons and honey badgers, can be drawn to the honey the bees produce, disturbing the hives to the point at which the bees themselves take flight and move out. King’s studies in Kenya, however, suggested that small cages can be placed around hives to prevent them from being disturbed by these other foragers. The dry seasons, too, can dampen bee populations in hives as they flee and search for moister climes. But the elephant intellect, says Gonçalves, proves the biggest obstacle to a static set of beehive fences. They have not, for instance, deterred clever elephants from seeking out alternative crossing points and taking up near-permanent residence in the buffer zone. Indeed, Stalmans says, sometimes a traditional, or even electric, fence is the best resort—but even those prove, over time, to be no match to an elephant’s keen navigational sense. Just as they are smart enough not to get stung or shocked, they’re also smart enough to simply chart routes around most human-made impediments and find their way to nearby farm settlements. It is here that Gonçalves [joins efforts]( with Gorongosa’s [human-wildlife coexistence team](. Together, they chart the peregrinations of known elephant families that have been collared with GPS devices and can thereby spot areas of potential human-elephant conflict before they erupt. Charles Digges is an environmental journalist and researcher who edits Bellona.org, the website of the Norwegian environmental group Bellona. Lead photo by Kevin Berger The Nautilus Gorongosa Series is published in partnership with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute’s Science and Educational Media Group. More from Nautilus: • [A plastic oasis in the sea]( • [A fragile equilibrium]( Experience the endless possibilities and deep human connections that science offers [JOIN TODAY]( [“](Understanding ourselves as primates has never been more important.[”]( Nautilus reader Brandon Brown (@brandonbobbrown) reacts to Max Beilby’s story, [“Clever Apes in the Modern Workplace”]( FACTS SO ROMANTIC The Best Things We Learned Today In an experiment in Mozambique, chili-soaked twine fences reduced elephant river crossings by 80 percent—while beehive fences thwarted a whopping 95 percent of cross-river forays by the animals. [Nautilus→]( Several male frogs often cling to one female, forming “mating balls” in the water, which can drown the female. [Nautilus→]( Aeromonas bacteria can easily trade their genetic material with other bacteria present in the environment—and in that process acquire some genes that can make them far more dangerous to accidentally consume. [Nautilus→]( Many organisms, like blue button jellies, thrive in the ocean region now known as the Great Pacific Garbage Patch. [Nautilus→]( The Temple of Bel, the center of religious life in early Palmyra destroyed by ISIS, is open for virtual exploration thanks to tourist photos and a digital resurrection of the site. [Nautilus→]( With artistic training or brain stimulation, we could look beneath the intrinsic nature of qualia to see the raw associations that make them up, just as a musician hears the individual components in what, to most fans, is a wall of sound. [Nautilus→]( EXCLUSIVE MEMBER CONTENT | [Explore Memberships→]( [Support Independent Science Journalism]( [Join a community of curious minds]( and get more of Nautilus’ award-winning writing every month. As a member, you’ll receive unlimited, ad-free access to Nautilus’ unique brand of independent science journalism. Available both online and in print. [JOIN NOW]( P.S. The logician and mathematician Alfred Tarski died on this day in 1983. In 1925, Tarski revived the squaring-the-circle problem, asking whether one could accomplish the task by chopping a circle into a finite number of pieces that could be moved within a plane and reassembled into a square of equal area—an approach known as equidecomposition. It wasn’t until 1990, Steve Nadis wrote, that [his question was answered with a resounding, “Yes.”]( Today’s newsletter was written by Brian Gallagher Thanks for reading. [Tell us](mailto:brian.gallagher@nautil.us?subject=&body=) your thoughts on today’s note. Plus, [browse our archive]( of past print issues, and inspire a friend to sign up for [the Nautilus newsletter.]( [Facebook]( [Twitter]( [Instagram]( Copyright © 2023 NautilusNext, All rights reserved.You were subscribed to the newsletter from [nautil.us](. Our mailing address is: NautilusNext360 W 36th Street, 7S,New York, NY 10018 Don't want to hear from us anymore? [Unsubscribe](

EDM Keywords (236)

written whether week website way water want wall wait villages vegetation university unison twine tusks turns trying trunk tranquilized trampled trample traditional traded town total today time tilled thursday thousands thoughts terrify temple task taking swat surrounded surround surprisingly sure subscribed subscribe stumbling study studies strung string stinger steal startle squaring square speakers sound solution size simple silos sign sides shocked series send seeking see search sea science romantic river results result rest resources researchers researcher recovery reassembled rapidly ransack raged question qualia published prove primates prevent power population ponder point plots plane pieces peregrinations people penetrate peaceful partnership park others open online offering observations numbers number night newsletter never nautilus nature mozambique move mouth mount methods member match mammal making make lying lolloping logician little leads latest known knock king kind kenya keep july isis intervene inspire insect impact hurts humans honey home hives help hear heads guns grew gorongosa go get genes friend found forays foragers forage flee flashlights fireworks find field fences fence farmers far fans family facts eyes experiment even european equidecomposition environment enough elephants elephant easier drown drawn dollars disturbed director destroy decimate dead day dangerous culminated crops croplands courtesy course could control confrontations conflict community colleagues collared coauthors circle chopping chilis check chart center cause came buzz bushes buckets boxes bel beira began bees bee baboons attack archive apt answered animals also acts action 773 50 2017 2012 200 1983 1977

Marketing emails from nautil.us

View More
Sent On

03/12/2024

Sent On

03/11/2024

Sent On

29/10/2024

Sent On

18/10/2024

Sent On

08/10/2024

Sent On

06/10/2024

Email Content Statistics

Subscribe Now

Subject Line Length

Data shows that subject lines with 6 to 10 words generated 21 percent higher open rate.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Words

The more words in the content, the more time the user will need to spend reading. Get straight to the point with catchy short phrases and interesting photos and graphics.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Images

More images or large images might cause the email to load slower. Aim for a balance of words and images.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Time to Read

Longer reading time requires more attention and patience from users. Aim for short phrases and catchy keywords.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Predicted open rate

Subscribe Now

Spam Score

Spam score is determined by a large number of checks performed on the content of the email. For the best delivery results, it is advised to lower your spam score as much as possible.

Subscribe Now

Flesch reading score

Flesch reading score measures how complex a text is. The lower the score, the more difficult the text is to read. The Flesch readability score uses the average length of your sentences (measured by the number of words) and the average number of syllables per word in an equation to calculate the reading ease. Text with a very high Flesch reading ease score (about 100) is straightforward and easy to read, with short sentences and no words of more than two syllables. Usually, a reading ease score of 60-70 is considered acceptable/normal for web copy.

Subscribe Now

Technologies

What powers this email? Every email we receive is parsed to determine the sending ESP and any additional email technologies used.

Subscribe Now

Email Size (not include images)

Font Used

No. Font Name
Subscribe Now

Copyright © 2019–2025 SimilarMail.