Newsletter Subject

The George Santos Syndrome

From

nautil.us

Email Address

newsletters@nautil.us

Sent On

Sun, Aug 20, 2023 11:03 AM

Email Preheader Text

Why people believe their own lies. Plus: why Darwin admired the humble earthworm; destroying an idea

Why people believe their own lies. Plus: why Darwin admired the humble earthworm; destroying an idea is a path to progress; and more. [View in browser]( [Join Nautilus]( EDITORS' CHOICE Did a friend forward this? [Subscribe here.]( This Sunday, your FREE member newsletter includes one full story, by the writer Clayton Dalton, below. Enjoy! PSYCHOLOGY The George Santos Syndrome Why people believe their own lies. BY CLAYTON DALTON In December 2022, The New York Times [reported]( that George Santos, the newly elected Republican Congressman from New York, wasn’t who he said he was. Santos presented himself as an experienced financial professional and entrepreneur, with a degree from New York University and stints at CitiBank and Goldman Sachs. He suggested that his mother’s death was related to 9/11. He implied that his grandparents were Jewish and survived the Holocaust. But neither NYU, nor Goldman, nor CitiBank had any record of him. His mother was in [Brazil]( in 2001. His [grandparents]( were, by all accounts, Brazilian Catholics. Santos has since [admitted]( to “embellishing his resume” but he continues to [maintain]( that he has Jewish heritage, and that his mother was in the World Trade Center on 9/11. “I stay convinced that that’s the truth,” he has said. Lying and deception are perennials of politics. Machiavelli argued in the 16th century that sometimes a leader must act as a “great pretender and dissembler.” It’s easy to see such politicians as fundamentally cynical. They seem to lie in a Machiavellian attempt to win power and control. Often, no doubt, that’s true. But cognitive science suggests the possibility of something more disconcerting at play: You really can believe your own lies. Insight into the nature of memory, based on experimental work, suggests that your awareness of the truth can bend over time. Lies can feel like the truth and so, to you, they are the truth. You’re no longer deceiving others. Instead, your brain is deceiving you. Your're no longer deceiving others. Your brain is deceiving you. In the 1970s, the psychologist Elizabeth Loftus began a series of experiments that upended the conventional view of memory. Previously memory was thought to function like a tape recorder. Information was permanently stored in the brain and retrieved at will. Loftus had a hunch that this wasn’t how memory worked at all. She suspected that memory might be far more malleable, and more subject to revision and distortion, than anyone suspected. In one of her earliest [studies]( Loftus and her collaborators showed participants a slide sequence of a vehicle accident involving a stop sign. Afterward, some participants were asked about a yield sign, even though there had been no yield sign. Then, on a subsequent memory test, participants were asked whether they had seen a stop sign or a yield sign. In the control group, which hadn’t been asked the misleading question about a yield sign, 75 percent correctly remembered the stop sign. In the other group, only 41 percent did. The others wrongly remembered a yield sign. The interviewers had influenced, or even altered, the participants’ actual memories. Loftus corroborated this effect in dozens of studies, and later showed that she could [implant]( memories of entirely fictional events in people’s minds. In the 1990s, researchers began asking a different question. Loftus had shown that false information supplied by others could alter memory of an event. What if the misinformation came from yourself? Psychologists Maria Zaragoza and Jennifer Ackil designed one of the first [experiments]( to test this possibility. They showed a movie clip of two brothers and their adventures at a summer camp to nearly 300 participants, who ranged in age from first grade to college. They divided the participants into two groups and asked them questions about events that had occurred in the clip, as well as about events that had not, which the psychologists made up. The first group was told they didn’t have to answer questions about the fabricated events if they didn’t want to; most didn’t. The second group was told they must answer all the questions, which pressured them to make up details about the fake events. A week later, all the participants were tested on which details had occurred in the video and which hadn’t. The first group passed the test handily. The second group, which had been forced to invent things about events they never witnessed, misremembered their fabrications as real, the psychologists explained. A BAD TURN: In the past six years, says cognitive psychologist Quin Chrobak, who researches distortion in memory, Americans’ divorce from facts and truth has led to a burst of interest among psychologists in “how to correct false beliefs.” Evidence is mounting, but solutions won’t be easy. Image by northallertonman / Shutterstock. In the early 2000s, two other researchers published studies on the effects of lying on memory. Kerri True, a psychology professor, [showed]( a group of undergraduates a video of a staged robbery. Part of the group was instructed to fabricate a description of the robber. One week later, all the students were asked to answer questions about what they had seen in the video. True found that fabrication corroded memory in two distinct ways. “Those who lied about the description of this character later forgot more of the [actual] details about the character,” True told me. Those students were also more likely to misremember their fabricated details as having appeared in the video. It was as though lying cemented the false details in their memory, at the expense of the real ones. Danielle Polage, a psychologist and former student of Elizabeth Loftus, conducted a test that tried to measure the effect of lying in order to deceive, rather than simply making something up. She [recruited]( a group of students, who completed a questionnaire ranking the probability that they had experienced certain life events as children, like whether they had ever been hospitalized, or lost in a mall. Two weeks later, the students were given a list of seven life events they indicated had happened to them, and one event that had not. They were instructed to tell convincing, detailed stories to an interviewer about all the events, including the one they hadn’t experienced. [Like the story? Join Nautilus today]( A week later, the students were asked to fill out the questionnaire again. For most of the students, lying about an event strengthened their memory that such an event had never occurred. But for a sizable minority, lying about the event led them to indicate that the event had definitely happened to them. Polage had essentially planted an entirely false recollection in their memories without saying a word. She published two more studies, in [2012]( and [2017]( on the effect of deliberate lying on memory, and again found that lying about a childhood event that hadn’t happened could lead to the development of a false memory of the event. In 2008, Zaragoza and cognitive psychologist Quin Chrobak asked another [group]( of undergraduates to watch the video clip of two brothers at a summer camp. This time, some participants were told to fabricate a fictitious event that had not occurred in the video. After one week, participants were given a yes-no questionnaire testing their memory of which events had appeared in the video, and eight weeks later they were asked to provide a summary of the video. At one week, some of the fabricators had already begun to believe that their made-up events had really happened. At eight weeks, when the students were asked to describe the video, the fabricators incorporated the events they had invented into their descriptions more than 60 percent of the time. Even the students who identified their fabricated events as imaginary at one week made this mistake after eight weeks. Strikingly, their lie felt truer as time passed. In the beginning, the participants clearly knew they had made up an event. “But when you test their memory later, you can get error rates over 50 percent,” Chrobak told me. Although these studies used different methods, they all found a remarkably consistent effect of fabrication and lying on memory. A leading explanation has to do with how the brain categorizes memory. In 1993, three psychologists published a [paradigm]( for understanding how the brain keeps track of a memory’s origin. They argued that, in fact, it doesn’t keep track at all. “People do not typically directly retrieve an abstract tag or label that specifies a memory’s source,” the authors wrote. Instead, the brain unconsciously assesses a memory for clues to decide whether the memory was internally generated—such as a lie—or externally generated, as with an actual experience. And among the latter, whether information originated from a trustworthy source or not, or from one person rather than another. In short, our brains consider the overall texture of a memory, through unconscious processes, in determining its likely source and validity. These unconscious processes, the authors argued, ultimately influence our development and expression of knowledge and belief, and color our engagement with the world. What clues does the brain use to make these unconscious judgments? “When you’re trying to figure out a source for a memory,” Polage said, referring to unconscious processes in the brain, “you look at a few things. First, perceptual. ‘Can I actually hear someone’s voice speaking to me?’ Then, context. ‘I should remember where it was. I should remember who was there.’ And then there’s emotion. ‘I remember walking in and I was the only one who wasn’t dressed up; I remember feeling really awkward.’” If a memory contains such richness, then the brain is more likely to determine that it corresponds to a real event. Lying cements the false details at the expense of the real ones. Polage was personifying processes that transpire beneath the veneer of consciousness. What enters awareness, ultimately, is the subsequent sense of where a memory came from. If our brains simply tagged every memory with a label specifying its origin, and filed them together, then memory might be less prone to error. But that seems not to be the case. Because we are largely unaware of these cognitive processes, they can be susceptible to mistakes—especially if we happen to embellish a lie with the sort of characteristics that the brain uses to parse what’s real from what isn’t. We can corrupt these circuits without any recognition that we have done so. If you tell a lie, the source-monitoring framework should identify the lie as a lie when you remember it later. That’s not a conscious process; we’re not choosing to remember or neglect that something we said wasn’t true. We simply remember the lie as a lie, most of the time, because of the texture and shape of the memory as assessed by subterranean levels of cognition. A lie likely has less perceptual and contextual detail, because you invented it rather than experienced it. Your brain notices that. The associated cognitive processes are different too, since creating a lie has been shown to be more demanding than telling the truth. Your brain also notices that. Your experience of this process is just the experience of correctly remembering that when you told your mother in high school that you spent the night at John’s house instead of Suzy’s, you were lying. But we can corrode this process by telling a lie in a certain way. “When you’re fabricating lots of details, you’re going to be more likely to see source errors,” True explained to me, and “the more plausible the detail, the more likely you are to get confused about the source of it.” In other words, the more you embellish the lie, and the more believable you make it, the more likely you are to confuse yourself later that it truly happened. If you created a whole litany of things that occurred at John’s house that night in high school, the better to deceive your mother, then you are more likely to misremember that lie as the truth in the future. Chrobak said that if a lie or fabrication provides an explanation for something, it’s more likely to become confused with what’s true. “People are causal monsters,” he told me. “We love knowing why things happen,” and if we don’t have an explanation for something, we “like to fill in the gaps.” The pressing human need to fill those gaps, Chrobak said, might also pertain to beliefs we hold about ourselves. After his arraignment on 13 criminal counts, Santos told reporters he believed he was innocent. “I will not resign,” he [said]( “I’m gonna fight the witch hunt.” If Santos sees himself as persecuted, then his lies might take on even greater significance for himself—they become proof of his innocence. Another important factor underlying this effect is repetition. “If I tell the lie to multiple people,” True explained, “I’m rehearsing the lie.” And rehearsing a lie seems to enhance it. “The more you repeat something,” Chrobak said, “the more you actively imagine it, the more detailed and vivid it becomes,” which further exploits the brain’s tendency to conflate detail with veracity. Further, Polage explained, “the more you access a memory, the more likely it is to be altered.” If someone is lying, Polage said, then “every time they access the false memory, they add detail to it, they strengthen it, they become more confident in it, and they might become more confident that the false memory they are building is actually true.” But “it’s not only what you rehearse, it’s what you don’t rehearse,” True said. When lying, you’re also not rehearsing the authentic details. “If you don’t access a memory,” Polage said, “the level of detail in the memory fades. You’re strengthening the false memory and weakening the real one.” I suggested to True that because memory is essentially reconstructed or reassembled whenever we remember, fabrication might use similar neural pathways. If the brain uses the same circuits for both, it might be easier to confuse the two. “I think that’s a great way of putting it,” True said, mentioning that [some research]( suggests that the neural circuitry involved in imagining an event is also employed in remembering one. “This is memory functioning as it should,” Polage said, but “we’re messing it up” with lies. “We give the false memories the characteristics of true memories, and so we blur the line.” I asked Chrobak if he thought people in positions of power and authority, including Santos, come to believe their own lies. “Yes, absolutely,” he said. “This is on psychologists’ radars now. People need to talk about this and why this is happening. A percentage of Americans believe demonstrably false lies. Whether they’re their own or politicians’, figuring out why this is taking place is relevant to our democracy. It’s really important.” That’s no lie. Clayton Dalton is a writer in New Mexico, where he works as an emergency physician. Lead image: Win McNamee / Getty Images More from Nautilus: • [Why Darwin admired the humble earthworm]( • [Destroying an idea is a path to progress]( Experience the endless possibilities and deep human connections that science offers [JOIN TODAY]( “Where does your reality stop and lies begin?” Nautilus reader [Jordan Sullivan]( reacts to Clayton Dalton’s story, [“The George Santos Syndrome.”]( P.S. The astronomer Fred Hoyle, who (derisively) coined the phrase “Big Bang” for the theory of the universe’s explosive origins, died on this day in 2001. At Cambridge University, Hoyle mentored the cosmologist Martin Rees, who mentioned Hoyle in a story about the possibility our universe is an island in an archipelago. “Hoyle favored a ‘[steady state’ cosmos]( that was eternal and unchanging,” Rees wrote. “(He was never fully converted—in his later years he espoused a compromise idea that might be called a ‘steady bang.’)” Today’s newsletter was written by Brian Gallagher BECOME A MEMBER Join Nautilus and $10 Goes to the Gorongosa Restoration Project Illegal poaching and the 15-year-long Mozambican Civil War almost entirely destroyed one of Africa’s greatest treasures, Gorongosa National Park, reducing its large mammal population by as much as 95%. But there is hope. Hailed as Africa’s most exceptional wildlife restoration initiative, the Gorongosa Restoration Project is a 20-year public-private partnership to restore the park’s habitat, protect its wildlife, and support local communities. Nautilus is proud to tell the story of Gorongosa’s extraordinary comeback, the people fighting to protect it, and the flora and fauna that call it home. Our coverage spotlights the comeback of a peculiar beast almost poached to the edge of extinction, why scientists are using Gorongosa to understand how an ecosystem heals from the depredations of war, and how researchers are charting the extraordinary web of life in one of the most biodiverse regions of the planet. You can now do your part to support Gorongosa’s guardians and protect its rich wildlife and landscapes. Join Nautilus, and we will donate $10 of all new one-year digital and print + digital subscriptions to support the Gorongosa Restoration Project. Read about Gorongosa’s remarkable resurgence while supporting the people who made this possible. [JOIN TO DONATE $10]( Thanks for reading. [Tell us](mailto:brian.gallagher@nautil.us?subject=&body=) your thoughts on today’s note. Plus, [browse our archive]( of past print issues, and inspire a friend to sign up for [the Nautilus newsletter](. [Facebook]( [Twitter]( [Instagram]( Copyright © 2023 NautilusNext, All rights reserved.You were subscribed to the newsletter from [nautil.us](. Our mailing address is: NautilusNext360 W 36th Street, 7S,New York, NY 10018 Don't want to hear from us anymore? [Unsubscribe](

EDM Keywords (335)

yes written writer world works words word wildlife well weakening watch war want vivid view video veracity veneer validity upended universe understanding understand undergraduates two trying truth true tried told together today time thoughts thought think things theory texture tested test tendency telling tell talk suzy suspected susceptible survived supporting support sunday summary suggested subscribed subscribe subject studies students strengthening strengthen story stints spent specifies source sort sometimes something someone solutions sign shown showed short shape series seen seems seem see scientists said richness revision retrieved resume restore resign researchers repetition remember relevant related rehearsing rehearse recruited record recognition really real rather ranged questions questionnaire putting psychologist provide proud protect progress process probability pressured power possibility positions politicians polage play plausible planet persecuted perennials percentage people path participants part parse park paradigm origin order one occurred night newsletter neglect nautilus nature much mounting mother mistake misremember minds might messing memory measure malleable make maintain made lying lost look loftus list line likely like life lies lied lie level led later label knowledge john jewish island invented interviewers interviewer instructed inspire innocent influenced indicated indicate implied imagining imaginary identify identified idea hunch house hospitalized home holocaust hold hear happening happened happen guardians group grandparents gorongosa goldman going given give gaps friend found forced flora fill filed figure fauna far facts fact fabricators fabrications fabrication fabricate extinction expression exploits explanation experiments experienced experience expense ever events event eternal espoused error entrepreneur enhance engagement emotion embellishing embellish effects effect edge easy easier dressed dozens doubt done divided distortion dissembler disconcerting different development determining determine details detailed detail descriptions description describe depredations democracy demanding degree deception deceiving deceive death day created corrupt corrode corresponds continues context consciousness confuse confident completed comeback color college cognition clues clip citibank circuits choosing charting characteristics case called call burst building brazil brain blur better bend believed believe believable beliefs belief beginning becomes become awareness assessed asked arraignment argued archive appeared another among although altered also age africa adventures access 95 2017 2012 2001 1970s

Marketing emails from nautil.us

View More
Sent On

30/05/2024

Sent On

29/05/2024

Sent On

28/05/2024

Sent On

26/05/2024

Sent On

23/05/2024

Sent On

22/05/2024

Email Content Statistics

Subscribe Now

Subject Line Length

Data shows that subject lines with 6 to 10 words generated 21 percent higher open rate.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Words

The more words in the content, the more time the user will need to spend reading. Get straight to the point with catchy short phrases and interesting photos and graphics.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Images

More images or large images might cause the email to load slower. Aim for a balance of words and images.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Time to Read

Longer reading time requires more attention and patience from users. Aim for short phrases and catchy keywords.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Predicted open rate

Subscribe Now

Spam Score

Spam score is determined by a large number of checks performed on the content of the email. For the best delivery results, it is advised to lower your spam score as much as possible.

Subscribe Now

Flesch reading score

Flesch reading score measures how complex a text is. The lower the score, the more difficult the text is to read. The Flesch readability score uses the average length of your sentences (measured by the number of words) and the average number of syllables per word in an equation to calculate the reading ease. Text with a very high Flesch reading ease score (about 100) is straightforward and easy to read, with short sentences and no words of more than two syllables. Usually, a reading ease score of 60-70 is considered acceptable/normal for web copy.

Subscribe Now

Technologies

What powers this email? Every email we receive is parsed to determine the sending ESP and any additional email technologies used.

Subscribe Now

Email Size (not include images)

Font Used

No. Font Name
Subscribe Now

Copyright © 2019–2024 SimilarMail.