Newsletter Subject

Finding the Neural Correlates to Consciousness Is Still a Good Bet

From

nautil.us

Email Address

newsletters@nautil.us

Sent On

Thu, Jul 6, 2023 11:06 AM

Email Preheader Text

How the wager between a neuroscientist and a philosopher will keep paying off. Plus: reality is your

How the wager between a neuroscientist and a philosopher will keep paying off. Plus: reality is your brain's best guess; is consciousness more like chess or the weather; and more. [View in browser]( | [Subscribe to Nautilus magazine]( July 6, 2023   Did a friend forward this? [Register here](. This Thursday, your FREE member newsletter includes one full story, below, by neuroscientist Anil Seth. Enjoy! Then afterward, be sure to check out this week’s Facts So Romantic   NEUROSCIENCE Finding the Neural Correlates to Consciousness Is Still a Good Bet How the wager between a neuroscientist and a philosopher will keep paying off. BY ANIL SETH   In 1870, Alfred Russell Wallace wagered £500—a huge sum in those days—that he could prove the flat-Earther John Hampden wrong. Wallace [duly did so,]( but the aggrieved Hampden never paid up. Since then, a lively history of scientific wagers has ensued—many of them [instigated by Stephen Hawking](. Just last month in New York, the most famous recent wager was settled: a 25-year-old bet over one of the last great mysteries in science and philosophy. The bettors were neuroscientist [Christof Koch]( and philosopher [David Chalmers](, both known for their pioneering work on the nature of consciousness. Chalmers won. Koch paid up. Back in the late 1990s, consciousness science was full of renewed promise. Koch—a natural optimist—believed that 25 years was more than enough time for scientists to uncover the neural correlates of consciousness: those patterns of brain activity that underlie each and every one of our conscious experiences. Chalmers, a philosopher and therefore something of a pessimist by profession, demurred. In 1998, the pair staked a [crate of fine wine]( on the outcome. The bet was finally called at the [annual meeting]( of the Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness in New York a couple of weeks ago. Koch graciously handed Chalmers a bottle of Madeira on the conference stage. While much more is known about consciousness today than in the ’90s, its true neural correlates—and indeed a consensus theory of consciousness—still elude us. Einstein’s relativity isn’t the last word in physics. What helped resolve the wager was the outcome, or rather the lack of a decisive outcome, of an “[adversarial collaboration](” organized by a consortium called COGITATE. Adversarial collaborations encourage researchers from different theoretical camps to jointly design experiments that can distinguish between their theories. In this case, the theories in question were [integrated information theory]( (IIT), the brainchild of Giulio Tononi, and the neuronal [global workspace theory]( (GWT), championed by Stanislas Dehaene. The two scientists made predictions, based on their respective theories, about what kinds of brain activity would be recorded in an experiment in which participants looked at a series of images—but neither predicted outcome fully [played out](. Given IIT’s focus on specific causal structures in neuroanatomy, Tononi hypothesized that certain forms of neural activity would be observed during the experiment in the so-called “posterior hot zone” toward the back of the brain. But Dehaene predicted that GWT would dictate finding consciousness-related activity in the front of the brain, reflecting the emphasis of this theory on the “broadcast” of information throughout broad swathes of cortex. [The results of the experiment]( did not fully conform to either prediction. Arguably, IIT fared a little better than GWT but not to the extent that any reasonable person could say that GWT had been disproved. And another experiment from COGITATE is still ongoing, which could yet change things around. [Like the story? Subscribe to Nautilus magazine]( Though the adversarial collaboration failed to identify a clear winner, it was by no means in vain. It doesn’t matter that the data didn’t decisively refute (or indeed confirm) either theory. That was never going to happen. The theories are just too different in their assumptions and explanatory goals, and our experimental methods too coarse, to enable one theory to conclusively win out over another in any single set of experiments. What’s more, the COGITATE studies tested only limited aspects of each theory—indeed, the core claims of each theory regarding the basis of consciousness were not tested at all. What the COGITATE collaboration demonstrated was a new way of doing things in consciousness science. It brought scientists from different camps together to jointly figure out what challenges they must overcome to test their theories, and to collectively pursue the refinement of both theories and methods. Science, after all, is a process, not a destination. Consciousness remains a tough nut to crack. The findings of the collaboration remain extremely valuable. They will push forward the development of both IIT and GWT—and other theories of consciousness, too—by providing new constraints and new explanatory targets. The data are also interesting in their own right, painting a detailed picture of where and when in the brain information about visual experience can be decoded, with both frontal and posterior regions playing a role. And because the COGITATE consortium will make the data publicly available, other researchers will have plentiful opportunities to take advantage of it. The students in the various labs who actually ran the experiments deserve enormous credit here, as do the project coordinators. The experiment also taught us a lot about adversarial collaboration itself. As the Nobel Prize-winning economist Kahneman [has frequently noted](, adversarial collaborations are hard. The COGITATE collaboration sets an admirable precedent as the first of several supported by the Templeton World Charity Foundation, reflecting [the rich array]( of theories of consciousness on offer. (I’m [involved in one]( of them, pitting IIT against a theory called “active inference” which emphasizes the role of neural predictions in conscious perception). What I really like about the adversarial collaboration model is the requirement that theorists must state in advance how they expect the results from the proposed experiments to pan out. So if and when theories are subsequently adapted, it cannot be said that they predicted what actually happened all along. Such “hypothesizing after the results are known”—[HARKing]( for short—progressively denudes theories of their explanatory and predictive power. By mitigating this temptation, theory development can become more transparent and more responsive to challenging findings. Future adversarial collaborations could take this further by asking theory proponents not only to pre-specify their predictions, but to also pre-specify their confidence in each prediction as well as the extent to which each prediction is core to their theory. Adversarial collaborations also encourage theorists to design experiments that have the potential to be problematic for their preferred theory. Of course, this is what scientists should be doing anyway (a view [Karl Popper]( took to an extreme), but it is not so common in practice. The pressures of academia instead incentivize researchers to design experiments that will produce evidence in support of their ideas. (You might think I’m being uncharitable, but a [recent survey](of over 400 different experiments in consciousness science found the large majority were confirmatory, and delivered evidence in favor of their preferred theory.) Consciousness remains a tough nut to crack, and none of the current crop of theories will be the last word on this last great mystery—[not even mine](. Nor will pinning down [the neural correlates of consciousness]( by itself be enough to answer the deeper question of how consciousness happens. There’ll be many more iterations of theory and experiment before we reach the stage when decisive experiments can be done—experiments of the sort exemplified by [Eddington and Dyson’s measurement]( of how the sun bends light, which confirmed Einstein’s triumph over Newton. And Einstein’s relativity isn’t the last word in physics. Adversarial collaborations, even in an ideal form, are not a panacea. They will only ever be part of a scientific solution. Back in New York, Christof Koch—ever the optimist—was keen to double down on his bet about finally finding the [elusive neural correlates of consciousness](. I imagine Chalmers might take it. But maybe, with adversarial collaborations added to the mix, and with exciting new developments in both theories and experimental methods in the pipeline, another 25 years might just do the trick. Anil Seth is a neuroscientist at the University of Sussex, co-director of the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR) [Program on Brain, Mind, and Consciousness](, and author of [Being You – A New Science of Consciousness](. Thanks to Tim Bayne, Megan Peters, and Lucia Melloni for comments on early drafts. Lead image: Ivan_Nikulin / Shutterstock More from Nautilus: • [Reality is your brain’s best guess]( • [Is consciousness more like chess or the weather?]( Experience the endless possibilities and deep human connections that science offers [SUBSCRIBE TODAY](   [“]()[Maybe if we spent a little more time teaching kids how to think rather than what to think, we’d foster a generation that would drive the change our world so desperately needs.]([”]() Nautilus reader Theo Burkhardt ([@ona.theoretical.level]() reacts to Karen L. Kramer’s story, [“What Industrial Societies Get Wrong About Childhood.”](   FACTS SO ROMANTIC The Best Things We Learned Today [The pressures of academia]( incentivize researchers to design experiments that will produce evidence in support of their ideas. [Nautilus→](   [The holy grail of aerodynamic design]( is laminar flow—that sublime moment when the air closes around a moving object without leaving a turbulent wake. [Nautilus→](   [Future noise-canceling headphones]( might rely on plasma—nothing but ionized air and wires—to deaden an unprecedented range of sounds. [Nautilus→](   [Water makes]( a lithium-ion battery fire worse. [Nautilus→](   [A third of North America’s birds](have vanished. [Nautilus→](   [The swelling helium nucleus]( is a sort of mini-laboratory for testing nuclear theory because it’s like a microscope—it can magnify deficiencies in theoretical calculations. [Nautilus→](   P.S. The Bulgarian writer Georgi Gospodinov won this year’s International Booker Prize for his novel Time Shelter (translated into English by Angela Rodel). The book, [according]( to The New York Times, is about “a clinic in Switzerland that treats Alzheimer’s patients by recreating a happy period of their lives”—a setting that allows Gospodinov to explore “lofty ideas about nostalgia.” Simon DeDeo wrote about how nostalgia is, in fact, [a law of nature](: “The exact mathematical relations between nostalgia, irreversibility, and dissipation are elaborate, specific, and always a bit of a surprise when they fall out of the equations.”   Today’s newsletter was written by Brian Gallagher   BECOME A MEMBER [The Dark Side of Storytelling]( [Issue 49 of Nautilus]( features “The Comet Year,” in which emergency physician Clayton Dalton meditates on the nature of the divergent storytelling around the causes of COVID-19—and what our fractured standards for truth could mean for the future. [GET NAUTILUS IN PRINT]( Thanks for reading. [Tell us](mailto:brian.gallagher@nautil.us) your thoughts on today’s note. Plus, [browse our archive]( of past print issues, and inspire a friend to sign up for [the Nautilus newsletter](. [Facebook]( [Twitter]( [Instagram]( Copyright © 2023 NautilusNext, All rights reserved. You were subscribed to the newsletter from nautil.us. Our mailing address is: NautilusNext 360 W 36th Street, 7S, New York, NY 10018 Don't want to hear from us anymore? Click here to [unsubscribe](.

EDM Keywords (206)

year written world wires well week weather want wager view vain unsubscribe university underlie uncover uncharitable triumph transparent today thursday thoughts third think things theory theories tested test switzerland surprise sure support subscribed students story still stage spent sort since sign settled setting series scientists science said romantic role results responsive researchers requirement relativity register refinement recreating recorded reach rather question process problematic pressures predictions prediction predicted practice potential pinning physics philosophy philosopher pessimist patterns patients part panacea pan outcome optimist one offer observed nostalgia none newton newsletter neuroscientist nature much mix mitigating microscope member measurement means maybe matter many make madeira lot lives little like law lack known kinds keen iterations involved instigated inspire indeed images iit identify ideas hypothesizing hear hard happen gwt generation full frontal front friend foster focus first findings finding favor fall facts fact extreme extent explanatory experiments experiment expect ever enough english emphasizes emphasis einstein eddington dyson double distinguish dissipation disproved different development decoded deaden days data crate crack course couple cortex core consciousness confirmatory confidence common comments cogitate coarse clinic check change challenges causes case cannot broadcast brainchild brain bottle bit birds bettors bet become basis back author assumptions association archive anyway answer another always along afterward advance 90s 1998

Marketing emails from nautil.us

View More
Sent On

09/05/2024

Sent On

08/05/2024

Sent On

07/05/2024

Sent On

05/05/2024

Sent On

03/05/2024

Sent On

02/05/2024

Email Content Statistics

Subscribe Now

Subject Line Length

Data shows that subject lines with 6 to 10 words generated 21 percent higher open rate.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Words

The more words in the content, the more time the user will need to spend reading. Get straight to the point with catchy short phrases and interesting photos and graphics.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Images

More images or large images might cause the email to load slower. Aim for a balance of words and images.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Time to Read

Longer reading time requires more attention and patience from users. Aim for short phrases and catchy keywords.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Predicted open rate

Subscribe Now

Spam Score

Spam score is determined by a large number of checks performed on the content of the email. For the best delivery results, it is advised to lower your spam score as much as possible.

Subscribe Now

Flesch reading score

Flesch reading score measures how complex a text is. The lower the score, the more difficult the text is to read. The Flesch readability score uses the average length of your sentences (measured by the number of words) and the average number of syllables per word in an equation to calculate the reading ease. Text with a very high Flesch reading ease score (about 100) is straightforward and easy to read, with short sentences and no words of more than two syllables. Usually, a reading ease score of 60-70 is considered acceptable/normal for web copy.

Subscribe Now

Technologies

What powers this email? Every email we receive is parsed to determine the sending ESP and any additional email technologies used.

Subscribe Now

Email Size (not include images)

Font Used

No. Font Name
Subscribe Now

Copyright © 2019–2024 SimilarMail.