Newsletter Subject

Reality Is Your Brain’s Best Guess

From

nautil.us

Email Address

newsletters@nautil.us

Sent On

Sun, Jun 25, 2023 11:05 AM

Email Preheader Text

Your expectations form the way you experience the world. Plus: the secret messages in ancient storms

Your expectations form the way you experience the world. Plus: the secret messages in ancient storms; a supermassive test for Einstein’s famous theory; and more. [View in browser]( | [Join Nautilus]( EDITORS’ CHOICE June 25, 2023   Did a friend forward this? [Subscribe here](. This Sunday, your FREE member newsletter includes one full story, below. Enjoy! [READ NAUTILUS](   [NEUROSCIENCE]( [Reality Is Your Brain’s Best Guess]( Your expectations form the way you experience the world. BY GEORGE MUSSER   Andy Clark admits it’s strange that he took up “predictive processing,” an ambitious leading theory of how the brain works. A philosopher of mind at the University of Sussex, he has devoted his career to how thinking doesn’t occur just between the ears—that it flows through our bodies, tools, and environments. “The external world is functioning as part of our cognitive machinery,” he said. But 15 years ago, he realized that had to come back to the center of the system: the brain. And he found that predictive processing provided the essential links among the brain, body, and world. Clark lays out the theory in a new book, The Experience Machine: How Our Minds Predict and Shape Reality, which is remarkable for how it connects the high-level concepts to everyday examples of how our brains make predictions, how that process can lead us astray, and what we can do about it. I recently caught up with the personable Clark to talk about our predictive brains. He explained, among other things, what’s behind chronic pain, why we are susceptible to misinformation, and how self-affirmation really can help improve your golf game. Clark’s insights were as bright as the multicolored shirt he was wearing. SEE THE BIRDIE: Andy Clark, a philosopher and cognitive scientist, says he improved his golf game with instruction that enabled him to predict that he is going to sink his putts. Photo by Stuart Robinson. Could you summarize predictive processing? There’s a traditional view that goes back at least to Descartes that perception was about the imprinting of the outside world onto the sense organs. In 20th-century artificial intelligence and neuroscience, vision was a feed-forward process in which you took in pixel-level information, refined it into a two and a half–dimensional sketch, and then refined that into a full world model. But in predictive processing, perception is structured around prediction. Perception is about the brain having a guess at what’s most likely to be out there and then using sensory information to refine the guess. Over the last 15 to 20 years, principally with the work of[Karl Friston](, that account of perception exploded into an account of perception, action, planning, and emotion. Maybe this is the fundamental operating principle of the brain. If you ask what is a predictive brain for, the answer has to be: staying alive. Predictive brains are a way of staying within your viability envelope as an embodied biological organism: getting food when you need it, getting water when you need it. You can’t let your energy resources get so low that you can’t go out and eat more things to get more energy. As[Lisa Feldman Barrett]( points out in her work, we start to feel hungry and thirsty long before the bodily resources are at that level. Likewise, if you feel thirsty and take a drink, you immediately feel your thirst is quenched, but it takes maybe 20 minutes for any real biological quenching to happen. You felt thirsty too soon, and now you feel unthirsty too soon, and that’s how it ought to work. [Want more stories like this? Join Nautilus]( Will artificial general intelligence, if and when it comes, require the machine to have a stake in its own survival or some equivalent of bodily vulnerability? I’m on board with this idea that being driven to stay within your own viability envelope is crucial to the kind of intelligence that we know about—the kind of intelligence that we are. So I think that anything that looks like an artificially generally intelligent system is going to have that, too. The other element that I think is important, and that is not there in generative AIs such as ChatGPT, is artificial curiosity. Predictive-processing systems automatically have that. They’re set up so that they predict the conditions of their own survival, and they’re always trying to get rid of prediction errors. But if they’ve solved all their practical problems and they’ve got nothing else to do, then they’ll just explore. Getting rid of any error is going to be a good thing for them. If you’re a creature like that, you’re going to be a really good learning system. You’re going to love to inhabit the environments that you can learn most from, where the problems are not too simple, not too hard, but just right. What are some examples of how we become aware of our own predictive machinery in daily life? Our expectations alter the way that we experience and perceive our world.[The Dress]( was a really interesting case. Suddenly people realized there’s a lot of hidden variety in perceptual experience. Different lifetime experiences have instilled different predictions about where the light was probably coming from when The Dress was photographed. If you think the light was artificial, you tend to see the dress as blue. If you think it’s sunlight, you’re going to see it more like white. Even when new evidence is presented, we tend not to be moved by it. Phantom phone vibrations are another nice example. If you carry your phone around in your pocket enough, then at some time you’re probably going to get the feeling that the phone is going off when it isn’t. There’s stuff going on in your body all the time; there are fluctuations in the sensory signals. The brain can latch onto a very small thing and treat it as phone vibration. It’s a clear and compelling feeling. It’s as strong a feeling as it is when the phone’s really vibrating in my pocket. It seems to be going on in chronic pain—it might involve an otherwise innocent bodily signal being taken as evidence for bodily damage. Although at one time there was a standard structural cause for the experienced pain, over time the structural cause has probably diminished. The level of experienced pain should have diminished with it. But somehow the system has got locked into a prediction of that early level of pain. We are statistically sensitive sponges. If the predictive-brain story is right, then everybody’s experience is going to be different in ways that reflect their idiosyncratic life histories. My colleague[Anil Seth]( is running something called the[Perception Census](, getting people to do some tests online to probe this possibility that there’s a vast amount of undetected difference in human experience. I think predictive processing is going to ultimately give us something like a periodic table of experiential variation: all the neurotypical and the atypical cases and everything that lies in between, and all the variety within typical and atypical. It’s just a small number of basic elements—model-based predictions, their precisions, and the flow of prediction error signals—combined differently to give you all these different experiential profiles. To round out these examples with the most severe and tragic one, psychosis and schizophrenia, how would that be understood in the predictive framework? Schizophrenia is a huge label. But in terms of a core thing going on,[Chris Frith]( and Paul Fletcher suggested misfiring prediction machinery. Imagine that for some reason prediction errors are being falsely generated. At first, you would feel as if there was something odd about the world—a sense of salient strangeness. The brain has got to find a model that can accommodate these errors. Because they are being wildly generated, anything goes: control by aliens, tech corporations, the Internet. You might start to find false confirmation. If you think you’re being manipulated by the Internet, you might find messages appearing on the Internet that seem particularly salient. Even when new evidence is presented to somebody, they tend to not be moved by it. But no wonder they’re not moved by it, because to them it is not evidence at all. They have all these strategies to reinterpret it in a way that is consistent with the model—so-called cognitive immunization. It’s an effect that you also see in Marieke Jepma et al.’s work on pain. They showed that if you predict intense pain, the signal that you get will be interpreted as more painful than it would otherwise be, and vice versa. Then they asked why you don’t correct your misimpression. If it’s my expectation that is making it feel more painful, why don’t I get prediction errors that correct it? The reason is that there are no errors. You’re expecting a certain level of pain, and your prediction helps bring that level about; there is nothing for you to correct. In fact, you’ve got confirmation of your own prediction. So it can be a vicious circle. Do you think this self-fulfilling loop in psychosis and pain perception helps to account for misinformation in our society’s and people’s susceptibility to certain narratives? Absolutely. We all have these vulnerabilities and self-fulfilling cycles. We look at the places that tend to support the models that we already have, because that’s often how we judge whether the information is good or not. Given that we know we’re vulnerable to self-fulfilling information loops, how can we make sure we don’t get locked into a belief? Unfortunately, it’s really difficult. The most potent intervention is to remind ourselves that we sample the world in ways that are guided by the models that we’ve currently got. The structures of science are there to push back against our natural tendency to cherry-pick. Armed with this self-knowledge, how else can we hack our predictive machinery? Pain-reprocessing therapy works for a surprising number of people. A person suffering from chronic pain can begin to contemplate that it’s a misfiring pain signal rather than an important signal saying, “You’re about to damage your body in some way, stop!” It’s the warning light that is malfunctioning rather than your car engine. When people treat the pain as a misfiring warning light and push themselves, that feeds back, because they get evidence against the model that they had before. That seems to make the experiential pain go down. Push back against a misplaced prediction to let the real thing emerge. [Placebo]( responses are another example. You are told that something is going to relieve your pain and, in many cases, that can have effects of the same intensity as an opioid. But interestingly, you may have to train the placebo. There’s a particular Parkinson’s drug, apomorphine. Once you’ve trained somebody’s brain to know how the body would respond given apomorphine, then placebo apomorphine can bring about those responses. You can’t just bring about those responses by being told, “Hey, this is a good thing for Parkinson’s.” You need to have trained the brain to predict the right set of physiological responses. So the doctor can administer the drug and then withdraw it and replace it with a placebo? Exactly. If you started with a placebo, it wouldn’t work. In The Experience Machine, you suggest that self-affirmation can work wonders, too. It sounds woolly. How can it be so effective? It’s not surprising if you’re pushing back against predictions that were misplaced. Your self-predictions are based, perhaps, on a misplaced societal picture. You’ve got to push back against a misplaced prediction to let the real thing emerge. This self-affirmation is practiced over maybe a six-month period. These are typically not effects that you get just from saying to yourself, “You know, I’m really good at this,” just before you go into an exam. It’s not about just affirming the end point: “I’m really going to rock this exam.” It’s about expecting the trajectory on the way—the flow of feelings or actions that would constitute success. So you have to learn how it feels to be doing it right. Then, reproducing that feeling brings about the actions that constitute doing it right. It’s not going to work in cases where the right skill set isn’t already present. I play a bit of golf. If you’re about to try and make a putt, you’ve got to know how to make a putt before thinking to yourself, “I’m going to make that putt,” is going to be helpful. For a long time, all I knew was how it felt to putt badly. If you know what it feels like to do it badly, then that’s what you predict, and that will force you to keep doing it badly. You learn your mistakes. At some point, I was taught a different way of gripping the club, and it enabled me to learn the right set of feelings—a feeling of stability that I never had before. A certain hint or instruction can artificially put your body into a position where you can experience the right set of sensations, and at that point you can start to improve in a robust and repeatable way. How are you continuing to develop these ideas? The thing that most excites me right now is understanding how our predictive brains ratcheted themselves up over cultural time. We’re trying to explore that using archaeological records, vision science, and so on. There was this strange point in prehistory when there was an explosion of human creativity, coming after a huge period in which there didn’t really seem to be anything much like that going on. Maybe there was a material externalization that helped get all of that going. When we write something down, draw a picture, or make a sculpture, our thoughts have become objects that we can think about. And thinking about thinking is characteristically human. George Musser is an award-winning science writer and the author of the forthcoming book Putting Ourselves Back in the Equation, as well as Spooky Action at a Distance and The Complete Idiot’s Guide to String Theory. Follow him at [@gmusser@mastodon.social]( and [@gmusser.bsky.social](. Lead image: fran_kie / Shutterstock More from Nautilus: • [The secret messages in ancient storms]( • [A supermassive test for Einstein’s famous theory]( Experience the endless possibilities and deep human connections that science offers [JOIN NAUTILUS](   [“Mathematics is a medium of self-discovery.]()[”]() Nautilus reader Jeremy Fortner ([@pubmix]() reacts to Elizabeth Landau’s story [“Are All Brains Good at Math?”](   [Join Us at the MB Summit of Minds]( Immerse yourself in the [3-day ideas festival](with nature at its heart. Enjoy innovative sessions and workshops on the science of nature and nature-based solutions at the top of the French Alps. [GET TICKETS](   P.S. The oceanographer, author, and filmmaker Jacques Cousteau died on this day in 1997. He pioneered modern underwater diving and was a great popularizer of the wonders in the ocean. Anthony Adler, in a [story]( about the dying-seas narratives we tell ourselves, quoted Cousteau’s son, Jean-Michel, who said, “The face of our planet is the ocean. It is the largest ecosystem on our Earth. But the face of climate change is not the whale, the polar bear, the glacier, the rainforest or the desert. [The face of climate change is us.](”   Today’s newsletter was written by Brian Gallagher   BECOME A MEMBER [The Dark Side of Storytelling]( [Issue 49 of Nautilus]( features “The Comet Year,” in which emergency physician Clayton Dalton meditates on the nature of the divergent storytelling around the causes of COVID-19—and what our fractured standards for truth could mean for the future. [GET NAUTILUS IN PRINT]( Thanks for reading. [Tell us](mailto:brian.gallagher@nautil.us) your thoughts on today’s note. Plus, [browse our archive]( of past print issues, and inspire a friend to sign up for [the Nautilus newsletter](. [Facebook]( [Twitter]( [Instagram]( Copyright © 2023 NautilusNext, All rights reserved. You were subscribed to the newsletter from nautil.us. Our mailing address is: NautilusNext 360 W 36th Street, 7S, New York, NY 10018 Don't want to hear from us anymore? Click here to [unsubscribe](.

EDM Keywords (317)

written would world workshops work wonders wonder withdraw whale well ways way want vulnerable vulnerabilities view unsubscribe university understood understanding typically two trying try treat trajectory trained train top took told today time thoughts thirst thinking think things thing theory terms tend tell taught talk taken take system sussex susceptible susceptibility survival surprising support sunlight sunday suggest subscribed subscribe structures strong strategies strange story started start stake stability soon something somehow somebody solved society sink simple signal sign showed severe set sense sensations seems see sculpture science schizophrenia saying sample said round rock robust right responses reproducing replace remind remarkable relieve reinterpret reflect refined refine reason realized reality rainforest quenched putt push psychosis process problems probe presented prehistory predictions prediction predict precisions practiced possibility position point pocket play planet places placebo picture photographed phone philosopher perception perceive people part parkinson painful pain ought opioid often ocean occur nothing newsletter never neurotypical need nautilus nature moved models model mistakes misplaced misinformation misimpression mind member medium maybe may manipulated making make machine low love lot look likely light lies level let least learn know knew kind keep interpreted internet interestingly intensity intelligence instruction inspire insights inhabit information improved improve imprinting important ideas idea helpful hear hard happen hack guided guide guess gripping got good golf going go glacier given give get functioning friend found force fluctuations flows flow first find felt feels feelings feeling feel fact face explosion explore experience expecting expectation excites examples exam evidence everything everybody errors error equivalent equation environments energy enabled else element einstein effects effective effect eat earth ears drug driven drink dress draw doctor distance diminished different devoted develop desert descartes day damage crucial correct continuing contemplate constitute consistent connects conditions club clear chatgpt center causes cases carry career bring bright brain body board blue bit begin badly back author atypical asked ask artificial archive anything answer already affirming administer actions account accommodate 1997

Marketing emails from nautil.us

View More
Sent On

03/12/2024

Sent On

03/11/2024

Sent On

29/10/2024

Sent On

18/10/2024

Sent On

08/10/2024

Sent On

06/10/2024

Email Content Statistics

Subscribe Now

Subject Line Length

Data shows that subject lines with 6 to 10 words generated 21 percent higher open rate.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Words

The more words in the content, the more time the user will need to spend reading. Get straight to the point with catchy short phrases and interesting photos and graphics.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Number of Images

More images or large images might cause the email to load slower. Aim for a balance of words and images.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Time to Read

Longer reading time requires more attention and patience from users. Aim for short phrases and catchy keywords.

Subscribe Now

Average in this category

Subscribe Now

Predicted open rate

Subscribe Now

Spam Score

Spam score is determined by a large number of checks performed on the content of the email. For the best delivery results, it is advised to lower your spam score as much as possible.

Subscribe Now

Flesch reading score

Flesch reading score measures how complex a text is. The lower the score, the more difficult the text is to read. The Flesch readability score uses the average length of your sentences (measured by the number of words) and the average number of syllables per word in an equation to calculate the reading ease. Text with a very high Flesch reading ease score (about 100) is straightforward and easy to read, with short sentences and no words of more than two syllables. Usually, a reading ease score of 60-70 is considered acceptable/normal for web copy.

Subscribe Now

Technologies

What powers this email? Every email we receive is parsed to determine the sending ESP and any additional email technologies used.

Subscribe Now

Email Size (not include images)

Font Used

No. Font Name
Subscribe Now

Copyright © 2019–2025 SimilarMail.